Seabiscuit and Taboo

There once was a time when anthropologists (and sociologists) struggled mightily to explain taboos — they come in such a variety of shapes and sizes that this proved a difficult task. Though often associated with animist ideas and religious formations, taboos cut across cultural categories and appear in all facets of life. Even people who don’t think they have taboos (and consider them stuff of superstition) act in accordance with them. Given the universality of taboos, it seems unlikely that any one theory or explanation will suffice. This doesn’t mean, however, that they are inexplicable.

In this delightful piece, Boris Johnson imagines being a Martian anthropologist doing fieldwork among the British and French. He observes that the Brits consider eating horsemeat taboo, whereas the French love their Seabiscuit. To explain this difference between two societies that are otherwise so similar, he draws on Durkheim’s idea that taboos are used to bind people together and create distinct identities:

It’s all about control. This amusing species called Homo sapiens has only lately emerged from prehistoric savagery; and what the human race fears most is a return to that darkness and disorder. So over the millennia the human race evolved the idea of taboo, as a way of creating social cohesion.

Things were deemed to be nefastus, haram, forbidden. Individually and collectively, people developed little electric fences in the mind, and by agreeing on what was taboo they defined themselves; they defined themselves in opposition to others; and they helped to create a crucial sense of identity. By controlling small aspects of behaviour, society helped to control the big ones.

Taboo, you conclude, was important in the creation of order. With growing excitement you continue your researches, and you observe several characteristics of taboos. They may become detached from their original rationale – shellfish and pork are less likely to give you food poisoning these days – and yet still have great force.

Taboos are psychologically powerful since they affect inescapable features of human experience: diet, reproduction, sexual behaviour, menstruation, defecation and so on….

As you get back into your spaceship, you draw several conclusions for the paper on human taboo that you will present to your fellow Martian anthropologists. You decide that taboos vary from country to country because in a way it doesn’t matter exactly what the prohibition is: what matters is the fact of the taboo; and the process of agreeing – and changing – the boundaries of acceptable behaviour is central to what makes a society.

By this Durkheimian (and social-functional) rendering, the important thing about taboos is not their content or substance, but as Boris says, the fact that they exist. Nothing else really matters.

While there surely is truth in this assessment of taboos, there is also something odd about ignoring what people actually say about taboos and why they observe them. Durkheim’s social-functional theory is nice, but it completely disregards — or considers unimportant — what people say about taboos. It’s as if the anthropologist observer is the only one who really knows what is going on behind the curtain — everything else is illusion. In contrast to this condescending approach, there exists the possibility that taboos exist for the reasons that people say they do, and that taboos actually do what people think they do. There is a certain scholarly conceit in asserting that beneath all this, something else is really going on and we know what it is. I’m not saying this isn’t the case, but we shouldn’t be so quick to discount (or see beneath) the explanations that are given. This was a point powerfully made, in a different but related context, by Robin Horton in his article “Neo-Tylorianism: Sound Sense or Sinister Prejudice” (1968). If you haven’t read the article or Horton’s other work, including his classic on African traditional thought and western science, I highly recommend them. Sometimes things are what they seem, even taboos.

Postscript — In this Spiegel piece, Susanna Forrest surveys European horse-keeping practices which produce conflicting cultural and market incentives. These conflicts have deep and hidden roots, as Forrest sagely observes:

Hominids hunted and ate the ancestors of the modern horse over 500,000 years ago, but it all got a lot more complicated 495,000 years later when the first Bronze-Age Kazakh threw a leg over the back of a wild horse. Equines span bureaucratic and cultural categories, being both livestock, working animal and pet. This leads to knotty problems in a modern, industrialized food chain.


Did you like this? Share it:

One thought on “Seabiscuit and Taboo

  1. Sabio Lantz

    “struggled mightily” –> loved that phrase. :-)

    It seems that there is a part of the human neuro-network that evolved to set “taboo” by giving a feeling of repugnation or disgust so as to make the organism “avoid” the item. It seems a bit different from phobias which generate fear. Of course I don’t know this stuff by that is my lay-speculation.

    Phobias come preset, it seems, snakes, spiders and others perhaps. And either they disappear as we get older or they get worse.

    Taboo seems to come preset too: excrement , sex with those raised with you, dead things.

    In that ways, Phobias and Taboos were adaptive. But like all things, they can be manipulated and so using these for control purpose, other people hijack these functions and teach people to put other items in those categories.

    The Taboo module (I know you dislike that word) breaks down in some mental illnesses and the sufferer broadcasts taboo items instead of avoiding. Since religions use the taboo module, we can see mental illness in the USA with people screaming obscenities about Jesus and Mary, whereas in India, it would be saying vile things about Krishna.

    I guess this pointed to me that, a social-function analysis on some taboos is correct and that the content did not matter, but the taboos are often used as boundaries of “our tribe”. They are hijacked my social manipulators to go beyond safety, to become a clan tool.

    I remember in Japan while watching an American movie with a Japanese friend, he thought it funny that one character called the other “a dick”. To my friend, it was nonsensical — like calling someone “a finger”. Our victoria taboos, eh?

    Taboos there were meant to control sexual behavior, it seems.

    So it seems that Taboos can serve many function but that certainly, part of our neuro-network (“some module” is simple reductionist terms), is awaiting manipulation — for better or worse.

    That JSTOR site was a pain– it wouldn’t let me register unless I lied about my academic affiliation. Unbelievable.
    I tried to read the Horton article but it was painful and I gave up. You have to know the issues of that time and the various anthropology positions. I would need a quick bullet summary of the positions to help. But I imagine you offered this link to other anthropology types reading this post.

    So I guess I am confused, when is a Taboo just what it says it is?

Leave a Reply